Zuker final.indd ns.indd
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
doi:10.1038/nature05401
The receptors and cells for mammalian tasteJayaram Chandrashekar1, Mark A. Hoon2, Nicholas J. P. Ryba2 & Charles S. Zuker1
The emerging picture of taste coding at the periphery is one of elegant simplicity. Contrary to what was
generally believed, it is now clear that distinct cell types expressing unique receptors are tuned to detect each
of the five basic tastes: sweet, sour, bitter, salty and umami. Importantly, receptor cells for each taste quality
function as dedicated sensors wired to elicit stereotypic responses.
Our sensory systems are responsible for generating an internal represen-
The anatomical substrates and units of taste detection are taste-recep-
tation of the outside world, including its chemical (taste and olfaction)
tor cells (TRCs; Fig. 1). TRCs are assembled into taste buds, which are
and physical (mechanical, sound, vision and temperature) features. In
distributed across different papillae of the tongue and palate epithelium.
this review we examine recent advances in our understanding of the
How are the different tastes detected, and how is taste quality represented?
biology of taste, focusing on receptors, cells and the logic of taste cod-
In the simplest scenario, sweet, bitter, sour, salty and umami tastants
ing at the periphery.
would each be recognized by different cells expressing specialized recep-
Taste is in charge of evaluating the nutritious content of food and pre-
tors. Coding at the periphery could then rely on straightforward labelled
venting the ingestion of toxic substances. Sweet taste permits the identi-
lines (that is, independent sweet, bitter, sour, salty and umami signals)
fication of energy-rich nutrients, umami allows the recognition of amino
to transform tastant quality into neural signals (Fig. 2a). In an alterna-
acids, salt taste ensures the proper dietary electrolyte balance, and sour
tive view, and the prevailing model for the past two decades1–3, TRCs
and bitter warn against the intake of potentially noxious and/or poison-
were proposed to be broadly tuned across taste modalities (Fig. 2b, c).
ous chemicals. In humans, taste has the additional value of contributing
In this case, it would be expected that individual TRCs would express
to the overall pleasure and enjoyment of a meal. Surprisingly, although
different families of taste receptors, and that tastant recognition would
we can taste a vast array of chemical entities, it is now generally accepted
result from decoding of the combined activity of various classes of such
that, qualitatively, they evoke few distinct taste sensations: sweet, bitter,
broadly tuned TRCs (the ‘across-fibre pattern' of coding)4,5. The recent
sour, salty and savoury (or umami). Although this repertoire may seem
identification of cells and receptors mediating sweet, bitter, umami and
modest, it has satisfactorily accommodated the evolutionary need for
sour taste (Figs 3, 4; Table 1) has generated powerful molecular tools that
an effective and reliable platform to help recognize and distinguish key
can be used to devise rigorous tests to distinguish between these models
dietary components.
and establish the basis of taste coding at the periphery.
Figure 1 Taste-receptor cells,
buds and papillae. a, Taste buds
(left) are composed of 50–150
TRCs (depending on the species),
distributed across different
papillae. Circumvallate papillae
are found at the very back of the
tongue and contain hundreds
(mice) to thousands (human) of
taste buds. Foliate papillae are
present at the posterior lateral
edge of the tongue and contain a
dozen to hundreds of taste buds.
Fungiform papillae contain one
or a few taste buds and are found
in the anterior two-thirds of the
tongue. TRCs project microvillae
to the apical surface of the taste
bud, where they form the ‘taste
pore'; this is the site of interaction
with tastants. b, Recent molecular
and functional data have revealed
that, contrary to popular
belief, there is no tongue ‘map':
responsiveness to the five basic
modalities — bitter, sour, sweet,
salty and umami — is present in
all areas of the tongue6,8,9,32,78.
1Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Departments of Neurobiology and Neurosciences, University of California at San Diego, La Jolla, California 92093-0649, USA. 2National Institute of Dental and Craniofacial Research, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, Maryland 20892, USA.
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 288
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 288
3/11/06 5:21:09 pm
3/11/06 5:21:09 pm
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
Figure 2 Encoding of taste qualities at the periphery. There are two opposing views of how taste qualities are encoded in the periphery. a, In the labelled-
line model, receptor cells are tuned to respond to single taste modalities — sweet, bitter, sour, salty or umami — and are innervated by individually tuned
nerve fibres. In this case, each taste quality is specified by the activity of non-overlapping cells and fibres. b, c, Two contrasting models of what is known as
the ‘across-fibre pattern'. This states that either individual TRCs are tuned to multiple taste qualities (indicated by various tones of grey and multicoloured
stippled nuclei), and consequently the same afferent fibre carries information for more than one taste modality (b), or that TRCs are still tuned to single
taste qualities but the same afferent fibre carries information for more than one taste modality (c). In these two models, the specification of any one
taste quality is embedded in a complex pattern of activity across various lines. Recent molecular and functional studies in mice have demonstrated that
different TRCs define the different taste modalities, and that activation of a single type of TRC is sufficient to encode taste quality, strongly supporting
the labelled-line model.
Sweet taste
Humans and mice show some prominent differences in their ability
The sweetness of sugar and the pleasure it evokes are so familiar to us
to taste certain artificial sweeteners and intensely sweet proteins — for
that they almost seem to be physical properties of sucrose rather than
example, mice cannot taste aspartame or monellin. Notably, introduction
a representation of neuronal firing in the brain. This tight relationship
of the human T1R2 receptor into mice significantly changes their sweet
between sensory quality, positive hedonic value and behavioural accept-
taste preferences to a human-like response profile15, proving that species
ance richly illustrates how sweet taste detection and perception evolved
differences in sweet taste sensitivity and selectivity are a direct reflection
to help with the recognition of the most basic and fundamental sources
of T1R-sequence variation between species. How does a single receptor
of metabolic energy.
complex respond to such a wide range of sweet-tasting compounds,
The attractive taste modalities, sweet and umami, are mediated by a
from simple six-carbon sugars to guanidinoacetic acids and even large
small family of three G-protein-coupled receptors (GPCRs) — T1R1,
peptides21 and polypeptides? Recently, biochemical studies of human,
T1R2 and T1R3 — that is distantly related to metabotropic glutamate,
rodent and chimaeric human–rodent T1R2+3 receptors have shown
pheromone, extracellular-calcium sensing and γ-aminobutyric-acid
that diverse classes of sweet-receptor ligands actually require different
type B receptors6–15. These GPCRs assemble into either homodimeric
domains of the receptor complex for recognition22–24, thus providing a
or heterodimeric receptor complexes16, and are characterized by the
simple solution to this puzzle. Together, these genetic, functional and
presence of long amino-terminal extracellular domains that are believed
biochemical studies have amply validated the role of the T1R2 and T1R3
to mediate ligand recognition and binding17.
subunits in sweet-tastant recognition, and demonstrated the importance
The critical role of T1Rs in sweet taste detection and perception
of heteromerization in receptor function.
emerged from an ensemble of studies, including the characterization
Definitive proof that T1R2+3 is the principal mammalian sweet
of T1R expression profiles, the analysis of naturally occurring sweet
taste receptor was obtained from studies of
T1r2- and
T1r3-knockout
receptor mutants (and the identification of species-specific differences
mice15,25 (Fig. 3). Homozygous mutants for either receptor subunit show
in sweet taste preferences), functional experiments in cell-based assays,
a devastating loss of sweet taste — all behavioural and electrophysiogical
and the generation of genetically modified mouse lines.
responses to artificial sweeteners, d-amino acids and low to moderately
T1Rs are expressed in subsets of TRCs, and their expression pattern
high concentrations (up to 300 mM) of natural sugars are abolished15,25.
defines three cell types: TRCs co-expressing T1R1 and T1R3 (T1R1+3
However, these animals retain very small, albeit measurable, responses
cells), TRCs co-expressing T1R2 and T1R3 (T1R2+3 cells) and TRCs
to very high concentrations of sugars. Importantly, a
T1r2;
T1r3 double
containing T1R3 alone8. What do these cells do? More than 30 years ago,
knockout completely eliminated these remaining sweet responses15,
genetic studies of sweet taste in mice identified a single principal locus
unequivocally demonstrating the essential role of T1Rs in all sweet taste
that influences responses to several sweet substances18,19. This locus,
detection and perception. Unexpected corroboration of the fundamen-
known as
Sac, determines threshold differences in the ability of some
tal requirement of T1Rs for sweet taste came from the recent discovery
strains to distinguish sucrose- and saccharin-containing solutions from
that cats (all felidae from the common house kitten to the tiger) carry a
water19. The
Sac locus was recently shown by linkage analysis8,11–14,20
naturally occurring deletion in their
T1r2 gene26, providing a molecular
and genetic rescue8 to encode T1R3, thus implicating a member of the
explanation to the striking, and long-standing, observation that cats do
T1r gene family in sweet taste detection. Indeed, functional expression
not respond to sweets.
studies in heterologous cells revealed that T1R3 combines with T1R2 (T1R2+3) to form a sweet taste receptor that responds to all classes of
Umami taste
sweet tastants, including natural sugars, artificial sweeteners, d-amino
Most mammals are robustly attracted to the taste of a broad range
acids and intensely sweet proteins8,10. These results validated the T1R2+3
of l-amino acids15,27–29. In humans, however, just two amino acids
heteromer as a sweet receptor, and suggested that T1R2+3 cells are the
— monosodium glutamate (MSG) and aspartate — evoke the unique
sweet-sensing TRCs (see below).
savory sensation known as umami (whose Japanese characters can be
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 289
6/11/06 10:02:57 am
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
glutamate and l-amino acids15 (but see also ref. 25). Together, these
results firmly established the T1R1+3 heteromeric GPCR complex as the mammalian umami taste receptor and provided a striking example of heteromeric GPCRs radically altering their selectivity according to a combinatorial arrangement of subunits (sweet T1R2+3 versus umami
T1R1+3). They also revealed that sweet and amino-acid (umami) taste — two chemosensory inputs that trigger behavioural attraction — share a common receptor repertoire and evolutionary origin.
Bitter taste
In contrast to sweet and umami taste, which evolved to recognize a lim-
ited subset of nutrients, bitter taste has the onerous task of preventing
the ingestion of a large number of structurally distinct toxic compounds. Remarkably, despite the vastness of this repertoire, these compounds all evoke such a similar sensation that we simply know them as ‘bit-ter'. These observations suggest that bitter taste receptors are probably
encoded by a large family of genes, and that the bitter sensation evolved to allow recognition of a wide range of chemicals, but not necessarily to distinguish between them.
Bitter taste is mediated by a family of 30 highly divergent GPCRs
(the T2Rs)32,33.
T2R genes are selectively expressed in subsets of TRCs distinct from those containing sweet and umami receptors32, and are
clustered in regions of the genome genetically linked to bitter taste in humans and mice32–35. A large number of T2Rs have been shown to function as bitter taste receptors in heterologous expression assays36–40, and several have distinctive polymorphisms that are associated with significant variations in sensitivity to selective bitter tastants in mice36, chimpanzees41 and humans42.
Proof that T2Rs are necessary and sufficient for bitter taste came from
knockout and misexpression studies in mice. On the one hand, animals lacking a specific T2R (for example, T2R5, the candidate cycloheximide
receptor), exhibited a marked and selective loss of their ability to taste the cognate bitter compound43 (Fig. 3). On the other hand, mice engi-
Figure 3 Sweet, umami, bitter and sour are mediated by specific receptors
neered to express the human candidate receptors for PTC (phenylthio-
and cells. The traces show recordings of tastant-induced activity in nerves
carbamide) and salicin, two bitter substances that mice do not normally
innervating the tongue in wild-type and various gene-knockout (KO) mice
respond to, became vigorously averse to these two chemicals43. These
or cell ablation studies (Pkd2l1-DTA). T1R1+3 functions as the umami
results demonstrated that T2Rs are necessary and sufficient for selective
receptor, T1R2+3 is the sweet receptor, T2Rs are bitter receptors (T2R5 is a
responses to bitter tastants, and validated T2Rs and T2R-expressing cells
high-affinity cycloheximide receptor), PKD2L1 is a candidate sour receptor,
as the
in vivo mediators of bitter taste detection and perception. In addi-
and PLC-β
2 is the effector and TRPM5 the transduction channel of sweet,
umami and bitter pathways. Note the extraordinarily specific taste deficits
tion, the fact that the bitter taste responses of mice can be humanized
(red traces) in each genetically altered mouse line. Pkd2l1-DTA refers to
by introducing human taste receptors illustrated an important feature
animals expressing diphtheria toxin in PKD2L1 cells.
of T2Rs and bitter taste: selectivity and sensitivity differences to bitter compounds between species is likely to be a reflection of sequence dif-ferences in their respective T2R repertoires44,45.
translated as ‘delicious flavour')30, perhaps best exemplified in western
A remarkable feature of bitter-receptor biology was exposed by the dis-
cuisine by the taste of meaty broths. A salient feature of amino-acid taste
covery that most, if not all, T2Rs are expressed in the same TRCs32. This
in animals, and umami taste in humans is their impressive potentiation
implied that individual T2R-expressing cells may function as broadly
by purine nucleotides (such as IMP and GMP)31. This feature has been
tuned sensors for all bitter chemicals but might have very limited, if any,
cleverly commandeered by the food industry as a means of enhancing
discrimination. In fact, it would not be unreasonable to imagine that
the flavour of a wide range of products, and was expected to be a bio-
although animals must be able to detect many bitter compounds, they
chemical hallmark of the authentic umami receptor.
have no need to distinguish between them qualitatively. Indeed, recent
Cell-based expression studies have shown that the rodent T1R1 and
studies in mice have confirmed that T2R-expressing cells operate as
T1R3 GPCRs combine to form a broadly tuned l-amino-acid receptor9.
universal bitter sensors43,46, and that, although mice and rats can distin-
These results validated T1R1+3 as an amino-acid taste receptor, and the
guish differences in intensity between bitter tastants, they are incapable
T1R1+3-expressing cells as candidate umami-sensing cells. Interest-
of discriminating between them47. Of course, it would be unreasonable
ingly, in cell-based assays, the human T1R1+3 complex functions as a
to expect that different bitter TRCs express the same T2R proteins at
much more specific receptor, responding selectively to monosodium
identical levels, and thus individual bitter-sensing cells can be predicted
glutamate and aspartate (as well as to the glutamate analogue L-AP4),
to vary in their sensitivity to bitter tastants but still be able to respond
with sensitivity that recapitulates human psychophysical thresholds
to the full repertoire.
for umami taste9,10. In addition, as would be predicted for the genuine umami receptor, both the rodent and human T1R1+3 heterodimers
Signalling downstream of T1Rs and T2Rs
showed strong potentiation in response to purine nucleotides9,10.
Signalling cascades downstream of taste receptors have been the subject
Final proof that T1R1+3 functions
in vivo as the amino-acid (umami)
of intense speculation over the years, with most models hypothesizing
taste receptor was obtained from the study of
T1r1- and
T1r3-knock-
a surprising diversity of pathways and strategies48–50. This proposed
out mice15,25 (Fig. 3). Homozygous mutants lacking either the T1R1 or
complexity contrasted sharply with the demonstrated simplicity of the
T1R3 subunit showed an overwhelming loss of umami taste, includ-
signalling pathways of other senses, such as olfaction, in which hun-
ing all responses to IMP and behavioural attraction to monosodium
dreds of distinct receptors share an identical transduction cascade51.
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 290
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 290
3/11/06 5:21:32 pm
3/11/06 5:21:32 pm
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
Sure enough, recent results have demonstrated that the receptors for
hyperpolarization-activated cyclic-nucleotide-gated (HCN) channels71,
sweet, bitter and umami taste, although expressed in separate subsets
acid-sensing ion channels (ASICs)72, potassium (K2P) channels73,74
of cells8, all signal through a common pathway to transduce tastant
and H+-gated calcium channels75, as well as the involvement of Na+/H+
recognition into cell activation46 (but see also ref. 52).
exchangers76 and acid inactivation of K+ channels77. However, recent
The current data suggest that tastant binding to T1Rs or T2Rs acti-
genetic and functional studies have greatly simplified the quest for the
vates the heterotrimeric G proteins gustducin53 or Gα (ref. 54) leading
sour receptor by demonstrating that a member of the TRP ion-channel
to the release of the Gβγ subunits46,55 and the subsequent stimulation of
family, PKD2L1, demarcates sour-sensing TRCs78. PKD2L1 is selectively
phospholipase Cβ2 (PLC-β2)46,56. Activation of PLC-β2 hydrolyses phos-
expressed in a population of TRCs distinct from those mediating sweet,
phatidylinositol-4,5-bisphosphate to produce the two intracellular mes-
umami and bitter tastes78,79, further substantiating the cellular segrega-
sengers inositol-1,4,5-trisphosphate and diacylglycerol, and ultimately
tion of taste modalities at the periphery.
leads to the gating of the taste-transduction channel (the transient recep-
Proof that PKD2L1-expressing cells function as the acid receptors in
tor potential (TRP) protein TRPM5)46,57. As expected from this model,
the taste system came from conclusive genetic-ablation experiments.
mouse knockouts of gustducin58,59, PLC-β2 (refs 46, 60) or TRPM5 (refs
The targeting of diphtheria toxin to PKD2L1-expressing cells of the
46, 52) have major deficits in sweet, umami and bitter tastes (Fig. 3).
tongue produced animals with a specific and total loss of sour taste78
Importantly, salty and sour tastes remain unimpaired in all cases46, dem-
(Fig. 3). These results validated PKD2L1 TRCs as the sole acid-sensing
onstrating that these two modalities use a different signalling pathway
cells and implicated the PKD2L1 ion channel as the candidate compo-
and operate independently of sweet, umami and bitter tastes.
nent of the sour taste (pH) receptor78,80. The further demonstration that
Are other pathways important in the detection and perception of
these sour-deficient mice have normal salt responses indicates that salt
sweet, bitter and umami tastes? We would expect signalling molecules
taste also must be mediated by an independent population of TRCs (see
representing many different transduction cascades to be present in most
Fig. 3 and below).
types of cell, including TRCs56,61–66. However, their mere presence does
Although sweet, umami and bitter sensing are primarily required in
not imply that they must be involved in taste transduction50. Compre-
the taste system, acid sensing is also important in a number of other
hensive physiological and genetic studies will be required to assess what
processes, including the monitoring of CO levels in the blood81 and
role, if any, they have in taste signalling. It would be interesting if second
the internal state of the cerebrospinal fluid and brain82. Consequently,
messengers from a number of pathways modulate taste signals, both
it may be predicted that PKD2L1 might also function in other physi-
at the receptor and downstream levels, and thus provide a platform to
ological settings. Indeed, Huang and colleagues78 showed that PKD2L1
shape taste responses as a function of various cues and cellular states.
is expressed in a selective population of neurons contacting the cen-
Intriguingly, activation of the TRPM5 transduction channel was recently
tral canal of the spinal cord that fire in response to minor changes in
shown to be strongly temperature dependent67, at a range within the nor-
proton concentration. These results suggest that these neurons func-
mal function of TRCs (15–35 ºC). Talavera and colleagues67 proposed
tion as sentinels of cerebrospinal and ventricular pH, and bring forth
that this property of the channel may underlie some of the effects of
a surprising unity in the cellular basis of pH sensing in very different
temperature on taste detection and, ultimately, perception67. It would
be rewarding to engineer animals expressing TRPM5 channels with modified temperature profiles and determine the behavioural and physi-
Taste coding at the periphery
ological impact of such changes on taste responses.
Several electrophysiological and calcium-imaging-based studies in rats and mice have reported that individual TRCs are tuned to various taste
Salt and sour tastes
modalities4,83–85 and have proposed that encoding of taste quality at the
A number of studies have suggested that salty and sour tastants modu-
periphery must use an across-fibre pattern of activity (Fig. 2). However,
late taste-cell function by direct entry of Na+ and H+ through specialized
the discovery that sweet, umami, bitter and sour (and, by extrapola-
membrane channels on the apical surface of the cell. In the case of salt,
tion, salt) taste cells are segregated into non-overlapping populations
TRC activation is believed to be mediated at least in part by the entry of
expressing distinct receptors (Fig. 4) demands a revision of this model.
Na+ through amiloride-sensitive Na+ channels68,69. However, the identity
We review three lines of investigation demonstrating that the TRCs
of the salt ‘receptor' remains speculative and highly controversial68,70.
defined by T1Rs, T2Rs and PKD2L1 function as highly dedicated sen-
A broad range of cell types, receptors and mechanisms have been
sors for sweet, umami, bitter and sour tastes, strongly arguing, instead,
proposed to be responsible for sour taste. These include the activation of
for a labelled-line model of coding across all taste modalities.
TRP channel
Figure 4 Summary of receptors for umami, sweet, bitter and sour tastes. Schematic representation of taste receptors (and candidate receptors) mediating
four of the five basic taste modalities. Although not indicated in the figure, responses to high concentrations of sugars, but not other sweet tastants, are also
detected by T1R3 alone15. The grey T2R receptor is designed to illustrate the possibility that T2Rs, much like T1Rs, may function as heteromeric complexes.
Similarly, the grey receptor next to PKD2L1 depicts a PKD1-family member as a candidate partner78–80.
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 291
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 291
3/11/06 5:26:27 pm
3/11/06 5:26:27 pm
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
Table 1 Tastant selectivity of candidate mammalian taste receptors
aversion mediated by sweet- or bitter-sensing cells, thus strongly sub-stantiating a labelled-line model of taste coding at the periphery. A final
Class of tastant
Examples of tastants
corollary to these findings is that expression of a sweet receptor in bit-
ter cells should trigger behavioural aversion to sweet tastants, whereas
l-Glutamate, L-AP4,
expression of a bitter receptor in sweet cells should result in attraction
glycine*, l-amino acids*
to the bitter compound. Indeed, Mueller43 engineered animals express-
Nucleotide enhancers
ing a bitter receptor in sweet cells (Fig. 5) and these mice showed strong
Sweet T1R2+T1R3 Sugars† Sucrose,
attraction to the cognate bitter compounds. Thus, the ‘taste' of a sweet
or bitter compound (in other words, the perception of sweet or bitter)
Artificial sweeteners
Saccharin, acesulfame-K,
is a reflection of the selective activation of T1R- versus T2R-express-
ing cells, rather than a property of the receptors or even of the tastant
d-Phenylalanine, d-alanine,
d-serine (also some
proteins‡ Monellin,
Bitter, sweet and sour
Using a conceptually complementary approach to the functional rescue
of subsets of TRCs described above, Huang and colleagues78 eliminated
entire populations of TRCs by genetically targeting expression of diph-
theria toxin (DTA) to defined subsets of taste cells. Remarkably, animals
expressing DTA in T1R2-, T2R- or PKD2L1-expressing cells showed
extraordinarily specific taste deficits, with each exhibiting a marked
loss of only a single taste quality (sweet, bitter and sour, respectively).
Taken together, these studies reveal three fundamental features of taste
Other toxic/noxious
Quinine, strychnine, atropine
coding at the periphery. First, they prove the functional segregation
of individual taste modalities at the cellular level (as proposed in the
Citric acid, tartaric acid,
original receptor expression studies8,46,78). Second, they show the abso-
acetic acid, hydrochloric acid
lute requirement of T1R2-, T2R- and PKD2L1-cells for sweet, bitter
*Preferentially activates mouse but not human receptors.
and sour taste. Finally, they demonstrate that animals recognize and
†High concentrations of sugars, but not other sweet tastants, can also be detected by T1R3 alone15. ‡Activates human but not mouse receptors and does not elicit behavioural responses in wild-type
respond to taste cues (that is, encode and decode signals) without the
need for combinatorial patterns of activity at the periphery (across-
§About 30 T2Rs are involved in bitter-tastant recognition.
fibre models).
¶Mouse T2Rs; all others shown are human. There are 25 human and 35 mouse T2R bitter-taste receptors. For illustrative purposes we have included receptor–ligand matches for a number of de-orphaned T2Rs (for example, mouse T2R5 is the receptor for the protein synthesis inhibitor
The exciting journey from detection to perception
The discovery that individual taste modalities are encoded by different TRCs should make it possible to mark the connectivity pathway for
each taste quality individually. As a result, it should be possible to trace
PLC-β2 is required for sweet, umami and bitter tastes46,60, so
Plc-β2-
defined lines of information from the tongue to the cortex to under-
knockout animals are blind to stimuli from any of these three taste
stand not only where these signals go, but where and how they combine
qualities (Fig. 3). If individual TRCs were tuned to a single taste quality,
in the circuitry to choreograph taste and flavour.
then restoring PLC function to a unique population of TRCs in
Plc-
Two recent reports have provided promising avenues to explore
knockout animals (for example T2R cells) should restore taste to a single
the connectivity between TRCs and central neuronal stations. In the
taste modality (bitter taste in this example). By contrast, if these same cells were broadly tuned to sweet, amino-acid and bitter tastes, then restoring function to T2R cells (by expressing PLC) would restore taste
to multiple modalities. Recently, Zhang and co-workers46 showed that
mice engineered to rescue PLC-β2 function exclusively in T2R-express-
ing cells respond normally to bitter tastants but do not taste sweet or
amino-acid stimuli. This ‘selective rescue' experiment demonstrated
both that activation of T2R cells is sufficient for normal bitter taste
and that bitter taste is encoded independently of sweet and amino-acid
tastes, with TRCs not broadly tuned across these modalities.
Bitter and sweet
To investigate the basis of sweet and bitter tastant recognition and
Wild-type control
coding, Zhao
et al.15 and Mueller and colleagues43 engineered mice that
expressed a modified κ-opioid receptor (RASSL; receptor activated solely by a synthetic ligand86) in either sweet or bitter cells. Animals expressing
Preference ratio (%)
RASSL in sweet cells become selectively attracted to the synthetic-opi-
oid-agonist spiradoline, a normally tasteless compound, demonstrating
that activation of sweet-receptor-expressing cells, rather than the sweet
receptor itself, results in the perception of sweetness. More importantly,
Bitter tastant (mM)
these results showed unequivocally that activating a single cell type is
sufficient to trigger specific taste responses. Does the same logic apply to
Behavioural attraction and aversion are mediated by dedicated
taste-receptor cells. Targeted expression of a novel bitter receptor to bitter
bitter taste? Mueller and co-workers43 tested this idea by generating mice
(T2R-expressing) cells results in dose-dependent aversion to the specific
in which the same RASSL receptor was targeted to bitter taste cells. Such
bitter tastant (open blue squares). In marked contrast, directing expression
mice showed marked aversion, rather than attraction, to spiradoline.
of the same receptor to sweet cells produces animals that are strongly
Together, these results demonstrated that a combinatorial pattern of
attracted to this bitter tastant (filled red circles). Control animals lacking
activity (across-fibre pattern) is not needed to account for attraction or
the receptor (filled grey circles) are indifferent to the tastant.
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 292
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 292
3/11/06 5:22:28 pm
3/11/06 5:22:28 pm
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
first, Finger and colleagues87 showed that all sweet, bitter, sour, salty
et al. Different functional roles of T1R subunits in the heteromeric taste receptors.
and umami nerve responses are lost in
P2x2;
P2x3 (purinergic recep-
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 101, 14258–14263 (2004).
et al. Molecular mechanisms of sweet receptor function.
Chem. Senses 30
tor) double-knockout mice, implicating the purinergic agonist ATP as a
(Suppl. 1)
, i17–i18 (2005).
potential neurotransmitter in taste87. The availability of these taste-blind
et al. The cysteine-rich region of T1R3 determines responses to intensely sweet
mice may now provide an experimental platform to engineer animals
proteins.
J. Biol. Chem. 279, 45068–45075 (2004).
et al. Detection of sweet and umami taste in the absence of taste receptor T1r3.
with function in defined sets of fibres, and therefore track the response
Science 301, 850–853 (2003).
of selective ganglion neurons. In the second, Sugita and Shiba88 used a
et al. Pseudogenization of a sweet-receptor gene accounts for cats' indifference
genetically encoded fluorescent transneuronal tracer to help reveal the
toward sugar.
PLoS Genet. 1, 27–35 (2005).
27. Iwasaki, K., Kasahara, T. & Sato, M. Gustatory effectiveness of amino acids in mice:
circuitry linking TRCs to the brain88. Interpretation of the results of
behavioral and neurophysiological studies.
Physiol. Behav. 34, 531–542 (1985).
this study was complicated by several technical difficulties, including
28. Iwasaki, K. & Sato, M. A. Taste preferences for amino acids in the house musk shrew,
poor transmission of the tracer, the use of a single label for both sweet
Suncus murinus.
Physiol. Behav. 28, 829–833 (1982).
29. Pritchard, T. C. & Scott, T. R. Amino acids as taste stimuli. I. Neural and behavioral
and bitter pathways (thus necessitating comparison between animals),
attributes.
Brain Res. 253, 81–92 (1982).
and the lack of anatomical correlates for many of the labelled neurons.
30. Ikeda, K. New seasonings.
Chem. Senses 27, 847–849 (2002).
However, this type of approach89,90 will be valuable in helping decipher
31. Yamaguchi, S. The synergistic taste effect of monosodium glutamate and disodium 5'-
inosinate.
J. Food Sci. 32, 473–478 (1967).
the neural wiring for sweet, umami, bitter, sour and salty tastes.
et al. A novel family of mammalian taste receptors.
Cel 100, 693–702 (2000).
Everyone appreciates that taste perception varies according to con-
33. Matsunami, H., Montmayeur, J. P. & Buck, L. B. A family of candidate taste receptors in
text. For example, the addition of sugar to lemon juice masks its sour-
human and mouse.
Nature 404, 601–604 (2000).
34. Lush, I. E. & Holland, G. The genetics of tasting in mice. V. Glycine and cycloheximide.
ness without affecting its acidity. More importantly, our perception of
Genet. Res. 52, 207–212 (1988).
taste is significantly extended by other inputs — such as olfactory, visual
et al. Localization of a gene for bitter-taste perception to human chromosome
and somatosensory — as well as prior experience, satiety and hunger91.
5p15.
Am. J. Hum. Genet. 64, 1478–1480 (1999).
This indicates that combination and comparison across taste qualities,
36. Chandrashekar,
et al. T2Rs function as bitter taste receptors.
Cel 100, 703–711 (2000).
37. Bufe, B., Hofmann, T., Krautwurst, D., Raguse, J. D. & Meyerhof, W. The human TAS2R16
together with information from other sensory systems, must ultimately
receptor mediates bitter taste in response to β-glucopyranosides.
Nature Genet. 32,
converge to orchestrate the final percept in the brain. Although elec-
397–401 (2002).
trophysiological studies of the response profiles of brainstem, thalamic
38. Pronin, A. N., Tang, H., Connor, J. & Keung, W. Identification of ligands for two human bitter
T2R receptors.
Chem. Senses 29, 583–593 (2004).
or cortical taste neurons are providing important insight into the basic
et al. Bitter taste receptors for saccharin and acesulfame K.
J. Neurosci. 24, 10260–
properties of the central taste circuitry92,93, the inherent technical dif-
10265 (2004).
ficulties in obtaining such data (often resulting in small sample sizes),
et al. The human taste receptor hTAS2R14 responds to a variety of different
bitter compounds.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun. 319, 479–485 (2004).
have so far prevented the formulation of a true consensus view94. We
et al. Independent evolution of bitter-taste sensitivity in humans and
expect that molecular genetic and physiological approaches using novel
chimpanzees.
Nature 440, 930–934 (2006).
reporters and genetically encoded activators and inhibitors of neuronal
et al. Positional cloning of the human quantitative trait locus underlying taste
sensitivity to phenylthiocarbamide.
Science 299, 1221–1225 (2003).
activity, combined with functional imaging at single-cell resolution95–100,
et al. The receptors and coding logic for bitter taste.
Nature 434, 225–229
will be invaluable in helping to decipher how information flows from the
tongue to sensory integration centres in the brain, ultimately, to dictate
44. Shi, P. & Zhang, J. Contrasting modes of evolution between vertebrate sweet/umami
receptor genes and bitter receptor genes.
Mol. Biol. Evol. 23, 292–300 (2006).
45. Go, Y., Satta, Y., Takenaka, O. & Takahata, N. Lineage-specific loss of function of bitter taste
receptor genes in humans and nonhuman primates.
Genetics 170, 313–326 (2005).
Smith, D. V. & St John, S. J. Neural coding of gustatory information.
Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9,
et al. Coding of sweet, bitter, and umami tastes: different receptor cells sharing
427–435 (1999).
similar signaling pathways.
Cel 112, 293–301 (2003).
Erickson, R. P., Covey, E. & Doetsch, G. S. Neuron and stimulus typologies in the rat
47. Spector, A. C. & Kopka, S. L. Rats fail to discriminate quinine from denatonium: implications
gustatory system.
Brain Res. 196, 513–519 (1980).
for the neural coding of bitter-tasting compounds.
J. Neurosci. 22, 1937–1941 (2002).
Erickson, R. P. The evolution of neural coding ideas in the chemical senses.
Physiol. Behav.
48. Chaudhari, N. & Roper, S. D. Molecular and physiological evidence for glutamate (umami)
69, 3–13 (2000).
taste transduction via a G protein-coupled receptor.
Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 855, 398–406
Caicedo, A., Kim, K. N. & Roper, S. D. Individual mouse taste cells respond to multiple
chemical stimuli.
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 544, 501–509 (2002).
49. Kinnamon, S. C. A plethora of taste receptors.
Neuron 25, 507–510 (2000).
Smith, D. V., John, S. J. & Boughter, J. D. Neuronal cell types and taste quality coding.
50. Smith, D. V. & Margolskee, R. F. Making sense of taste.
Sci. Am. 284, 32–39 (2001).
Physiol. Behav. 69, 77–85 (2000).
51. Brunet, L. J., Gold, G. H. & Ngai, J. General anosmia caused by a targeted disruption of the
et al. Putative mammalian taste receptors: a class of taste-specific GPCRs
mouse olfactory cyclic nucleotide-gated cation channel.
Neuron 17, 681–693 (1996).
with distinct topographic selectivity.
Cel 96, 541–551 (1999).
et al. Trpm5 null mice respond to bitter, sweet, and umami compounds.
Chem.
Bachmanov, A. A.
et al. Positional cloning of the mouse saccharin preference (
Sac) locus.
Senses 31, 253–264 (2006).
Chem. Senses 26, 925–933 (2001).
53. McLaughlin, S. K., McKinnon, P. J. & Margolskee, R. F. Gustducin is a taste-cell-specific G
et al. Mammalian sweet taste receptors.
Cell 106, 381–390 (2001).
protein closely related to the transducins.
Nature 357, 563–569 (1992).
et al. An amino-acid taste receptor.
Nature 416, 199–202 (2002).
et al. Comprehensive study on G protein α-subunits in taste bud cells, with
et al. Human receptors for sweet and umami taste.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 99,
special reference to the occurrence of Gαi2 as a major Gα species.
Chem. Senses 25,
4692–4696 (2002).
525–531 (2000).
Kitagawa, M., Kusakabe, Y., Miura, H., Ninomiya, Y. & Hino, A. Molecular genetic
et al. Gγ13 colocalizes with gustducin in taste receptor cells and mediates IP3
identification of a candidate receptor gene for sweet taste.
Biochem. Biophys. Res. Commun.
responses to bitter denatonium.
Nature Neurosci. 2, 1055–1062 (1999).
283, 236–242 (2001).
56. Rossler, P., Kroner, C., Freitag, J., Noe, J. & Breer, H. Identification of a phospholipase C β
et al. Tas1r3, encoding a new candidate taste receptor, is allelic to the sweet
subtype in rat taste cells.
Eur. J. Cell Biol. 77, 253–261 (1998).
responsiveness locus
Sac.
Nature Genet. 28, 58–63 (2001).
et al. A transient receptor potential channel expressed in taste receptor cells.
13. Montmayeur, J. P., Liberles, S. D., Matsunami, H. & Buck, L. B. A candidate taste receptor
Nature Neurosci. 5, 1169–1176 (2002).
gene near a sweet taste locus.
Nature Neurosci. 4, 492–498 (2001).
58. Wong, G. T., Gannon, K. S. & Margolskee, R. F. Transduction of bitter and sweet taste by
14. Sainz, E., Korley, J. N., Battey, J. F. & Sullivan, S. L. Identification of a novel member of the
gustducin.
Nature 381, 796–800 (1996).
T1R family of putative taste receptors.
J. Neurochem. 77, 896–903 (2001).
59. Ruiz, C. J., Wray, K., Delay, E., Margolskee, R. F. & Kinnamon, S. C. Behavioral evidence for a
et al. The receptors for mammalian sweet and umami taste.
Cel 115, 255–266
role of α-gustducin in glutamate taste.
Chem. Senses 28, 573–579 (2003).
60. Dotson, C. D., Roper, S. D. & Spector, A. C. PLCβ2-independent behavioral avoidance of
16. Pin, J. P. & Acher, F. The metabotropic glutamate receptors: structure, activation
prototypical bitter-tasting ligands.
Chem. Senses 30, 593–600 (2005).
mechanism and pharmacology.
Curr. Drug Targets CNS Neurol. Disord. 1, 297–317 (2002).
61. Varkevisser, B. & Kinnamon, S. C. Sweet taste transduction in hamster: role of protein
et al. Structural basis of glutamate recognition by a dimeric metabotropic
kinases.
J. Neurophysiol. 83, 2526–2532 (2000).
glutamate receptor.
Nature 407, 971–977 (2000).
62. Rosenzweig, S., Yan, W., Dasso, M. & Spielman, A. I. Possible novel mechanism for bitter
18. Fuller, J. L. Single-locus control of saccharin preference in mice.
J. Hered. 65, 33–36 (1974).
taste mediated through cGMP.
J. Neurophysiol. 81, 1661–1665 (1999).
19. Lush, I. E. The genetics of tasting in mice. VI. Saccharin, acesulfame, dulcin and sucrose.
63. Bernhardt, S. J., Naim, M., Zehavi, U. & Lindemann, B. Changes in IP3 and cytosolic Ca2+ in
Genet. Res. 53, 95–99 (1989).
response to sugars and non-sugar sweeteners in transduction of sweet taste in the rat.
J.
et al. High-resolution genetic mapping of the saccharin preference locus (Sac) and
Physiol. (Lond.) 490, 325–336 (1996).
the putative sweet taste receptor (T1R1) gene (Gpr70) to mouse distal Chromosome 4.
64. Striem, B. J., Pace, U., Zehavi, U., Naim, M. & Lancet, D. Sweet tastants stimulate adenylate
Mamm. Genome 12, 13–16 (2001).
cyclase coupled to GTP-binding protein in rat tongue membranes.
Biochem. J. 260, 121–126
21. Danilova, V., Hellekant, G., Tinti, J. M. & Nofre, C. Gustatory responses of the hamster
Mesocricetus auratus to various compounds considered sweet by humans.
J. Neurophysiol.
65. Gilbertson, T. A. & Boughter, J. D. Taste transduction: appetizing times in gustation.
80, 2102–2112 (1998).
Neuroreport 14, 905–911 (2003).
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 293
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 293
3/11/06 5:22:55 pm
3/11/06 5:22:55 pm
NATURE Vol 444 16 November 2006
66. Avenet, P., Hofmann, F. & Lindemann, B. Transduction in taste receptor cells requires
et al. Conditional expression and signaling of a specifically designed Gi-
cAMP-dependent protein kinase.
Nature 331, 351–354 (1988).
coupled receptor in transgenic mice.
Nature Biotechnol. 17, 165–169 (1999).
et al. Heat activation of TRPM5 underlies thermal sensitivity of sweet taste.
et al. ATP signaling is crucial for communication from taste buds to gustatory
Nature 438, 1022–1025 (2005).
nerves.
Science 310, 1495–1499 (2005).
68. Heck, G. L., Mierson, S. & DeSimone, J. A. Salt taste transduction occurs through an
88. Sugita, M. & Shiba, Y. Genetic tracing shows segregation of taste neuronal circuitries for
amiloride-sensitive sodium transport pathway.
Science 223, 403–405 (1984).
bitter and sweet.
Science 309, 781–785 (2005).
69. Avenet, P. & Lindemann, B. Amiloride-blockable sodium currents in isolated taste receptor
89. Zou, Z., Horowitz, L. F., Montmayeur, J. P., Snapper, S. & Buck, L. B. Genetic tracing reveals a
cells.
J. Membr. Biol. 105, 245–255 (1988).
stereotyped sensory map in the olfactory cortex.
Nature 414, 173–179 (2001).
et al. The mammalian amiloride-insensitive non-specific salt taste receptor is a
90. Kuze, B., Matsuyama, K., Matsui, T., Miyata, H. & Mori, S. Segment-specific branching
vanilloid receptor-1 variant.
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 558, 147–159 (2004).
patterns of single vestibulospinal tract axons arising from the lateral vestibular nucleus in
et al. Hyperpolarization-activated channels HCN1 and HCN4 mediate
the cat: A PHA-L tracing study.
J. Comp. Neurol. 414, 80–96 (1999).
responses to sour stimuli.
Nature 413, 631–635 (2001).
The Brain and Emotion (Oxford Univ. Press, USA, 2000).
et al. Receptor that leaves a sour taste in the mouth.
Nature 395, 555–556
92. Katz, D. B., Simon, S. A. & Nicolelis, M. A. Dynamic and multimodal responses of gustatory
cortical neurons in awake rats.
J. Neurosci. 21, 4478–4489 (2001).
73. Lin, W., Burks, C. A., Hansen, D. R., Kinnamon, S. C. & Gilbertson, T. A. Taste receptor cells
93. Di Lorenzo, P. M. The neural code for taste in the brain stem: response profiles.
Physiol.
express pH-sensitive leak K+ channels.
J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2909–2919 (2004).
Behav. 69, 87–96 (2000).
74. Richter, T. A., Dvoryanchikov, G. A., Chaudhari, N. & Roper, S. D. Acid-sensitive two-pore
94. Spector, A. C. & Travers, S. P. The representation of taste quality in the mammalian nervous
domain potassium (K2P) channels in mouse taste buds.
J. Neurophysiol. 92, 1928–1936
system.
Behav. Cogn. Neurosci. Rev. 4, 143–191 (2005).
95. Zhang, J., Campbell, R. E., Ting, A. Y. & Tsien, R. Y. Creating new fluorescent probes for cell
75. Waldmann, R., Champigny, G., Bassilana, F., Heurteaux, C. & Lazdunski, M. A proton-gated
biology.
Nature Rev. Mol. Cell Biol. 3, 906–918 (2002).
cation channel involved in acid-sensing.
Nature 386, 173–177 (1997).
et al. Lhx6 delineates a pathway mediating innate reproductive behaviors from
et al. Basolateral Na+–H+ exchanger-1 in rat taste receptor cells is involved in neural
the amygdala to the hypothalamus.
Neuron 46, 647–660 (2005).
adaptation to acidic stimuli.
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 556, 159–173 (2004).
97. Miesenbock, G. & Kevrekidis, I. G. Optical imaging and control of genetically designated
77. Cummings, T. A. & Kinnamon, S. C. Apical K+ channels in
Necturus taste cells. Modulation
neurons in functioning circuits.
Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 28, 533–563 (2005).
by intracellular factors and taste stimuli.
J. Gen. Physiol. 99, 591–613 (1992).
et al. V1 spinal neurons regulate the speed of vertebrate locomotor outputs.
78. Huang, A. L.
et al. The cells and logic for mammalian sour taste detection.
Nature 442,
Nature 440, 215–219 (2006).
934–938 (2006).
99. Gogos, J. A., Osborne, J., Nemes, A., Mendelsohn, M. & Axel, R. Genetic ablation and
79. Lopezjimenez, N. D.
et al. Two members of the TRPP family of ion channels, Pkd1l3 and
restoration of the olfactory topographic map.
Cell 103, 609–620 (2000).
Pkd2l1, are co-expressed in a subset of taste receptor cells.
J. Neurochem. 98, 68–77
100. Brecht, M.
et al. Novel approaches to monitor and manipulate single neurons
in vivo.
J.
Neurosci. 24, 9223–9227 (2004).
et al. Transient receptor potential family members PKD1L3 and PKD2L1 form a
candidate sour taste receptor.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 12569–12574 (2006).
81. Lahiri, S. & Forster, R. E. CO /H+ sensing: peripheral and central chemoreception.
Acknowledgements We thank a group of extraordinary students, postdoctoral
Biochem. Cell Biol. 35, 1413–1435 (2003).
fellows and research technicians in our laboratories, who joined us on this
et al. The system of cerebrospinal fluid-contacting neurons. Its supposed role in the
wonderful journey of mammalian taste research beginning in the fall of 1997.
nonsynaptic signal transmission of the brain.
Histol. Histopathol. 19, 607–628 (2004).
N.J.P.R. is an investigator in the Intramural program at the NIH, NIDCR. C.S.Z. is an
83. Gilbertson, T. A., Boughter, J. D., Zhang, H. & Smith, D. V. Distribution of gustatory
investigator of the Howard Hughes Medical Institute.
sensitivities in rat taste cells: whole-cell responses to apical chemical stimulation.
J.
Neurosci. 21, 4931–4941 (2001).
84. Sato, T. & Beidler, L. M. Broad tuning of rat taste cells for four basic taste stimuli.
Chem.
Author Information Reprints and permissions information is available at
Senses 22, 287–293 (1997).
npg.nature.com/reprintsandpermissions. The authors declare no competing
85. Richter, T. A., Caicedo, A. & Roper, S. D. Sour taste stimuli evoke Ca2+ and pH responses in
financial interests. Correspondence should be addressed to C.S.Z. or N.J.P.R.
mouse taste cells.
J. Physiol. (Lond.) 547, 475–483 (2003).
2006
Nature Publishing Group
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 294
Zuker final.indd NS.indd 294
3/11/06 5:23:19 pm
3/11/06 5:23:19 pm
Source: http://biomed.brown.edu/arise/resources/docs/receptors%20and%20cells%20for%20mamalian%20taste.pdf
REPUBLICA DE COLOMBIA DEPARTAMENTO DEL META MUNICIPIO FUENTEDEORO MUNICIPIO DE FUENTEDEORO INVITACION PÚBLICA No. 045 de 2010 "EL SUMINISTRO DE LOS MEDICAMENTOS FORMULADOS A LA POBLACIÓN POBRE NO AFILIADA AL SGSSS Y POBLACIÓN EN SITUACIÓN DE DESPLAZAMIENTO DEL MUNICIPIO DE FUENTEDEORO
Balancing Brain Chemistry to Treat Depression By Liz Butler This article first appeared in CAM magazine Introduction It is taking a long time for the scientific community to fully accept that what a person eats can influence their mental state but in the last few decades progress in this area has been rapid. Taking a very basic view of the subject there can be no doubt that nutrition is intimately involved with mental health as the brain and its chemical messengers are ultimately derived from food. Convincing doctors used to the traditional approach of treating mental disturbance and depression (drugs or psychotherapy) to consider the nutritional treatment approach is more difficult than simply pointing out this fact. Fortunately there is now a large amount of research supporting the view that nutrition has a role to play in promoting mental health, this article will review some of this research. It is well established that neurotransmitter imbalances can lead to mental dysfunction and depression and in fact most drugs currently being used in this area of disease aim to restore chemical balance within the nervous system (1). As some of the research mentioned in this review shows, certain nutritional factors may be able to promote chemical normality in the same way as current pharmaceutical treatments but without the side effects associated with drug therapy. Within a discussion about depression there must be some mention of genetic factors as there is no denying that the risk of developing depression, particularly a severe form, is influenced by genetics (2). It is likely that certain people are born with a predisposition to biochemical imbalances within the brain and then an inadequate nutrient intake compounds the problem. Eventually the situation deteriorates until there is expression of disease. What this means however, is that even disease with a genetic component may possibly be reversed given the correct nutrients to balance brain chemistry. Brain chemistry The brain is composed of about 100 billion neurones, the cells of the nervous system that communicate messages to each other, making up what is termed grey matter. The processes that extend from the cells to meet up with other cells constitute the white matter of the brain. Amongst the neurones are cells called neuroglia. Their role is to support, protect, and repair the neurones. Neurotransmitters are chemical substances that pass between neurones relaying messages. Examples include acetylcholine, histamine, adrenaline, noradrenaline, dopamine, and serotonin. All of these are well-studied neurotransmitters, and the effects of too much, or too little on the mental state are well observed. In addition neuromodulators and neurohormones are further classes of chemicals that affect nervous function. Neuromodulators modulate signal transmission either pre- or post-synaptically and neurohormones behave like neurotransmitters but act at a site distant